Skip to main content

Trickery Vision Collection

"Unveiling the Enigma: Dive into the World of Trickery Vision" Step into a realm where reality bends and perception deceives

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Ouchi illusion

Ouchi illusion. The central circular area of perpendicularly orientated bars appears to move and float compared to the surrounding pattern

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Goblet illusion

Goblet illusion. This image is simultaneously a drawing of a goblet and two faces. When there are two competing interpretations of an ambiguous image the brain will often switch between them

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Penrose stairs, artwork

Penrose stairs, artwork
Penrose stairs, computer artwork. This is an impossible figure created by the physicist Roger Penrose and used by M C Escher in his illustration Ascending and Descending

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Impossible triangle, artwork

Impossible triangle, artwork
Impossible triangle, computer artwork. This is an impossible figure created by the physicist Roger Penrose. Impossible figures are objects that can be drawn but not created

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Penrose stairs, artwork

Penrose stairs, artwork
Penrose stairs. Computer artwork of Einstein characters climbing a set of Penrose stairs. This is an impossible figure created by the physicist Roger Penrose

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Wundt illusion

Wundt illusion. This illusion was created by the psychologist Willhelm Wundt in the 19th century. It is the exact opposite of the Hering illusion

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Ponzos illusion

Ponzos illusion. The vertical red line to the left of the image appears to be smaller than the one to the right. In fact they are the same length

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Ehrenstein illusion, square in circles

Ehrenstein illusion, square in circles
Ehrensteins illusion - a square in concentric circles. This is one of a number of illusions studied by Walter Ehrenstein. The sides of a square placed inside concentric circles appear to curve

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Impossible shape

Impossible shape, computer artwork

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Square illusion - subdivision

Square illusion - subdivision leads to apparent enlargement. There are three squares of identical area. However, the middle square appears to be wider than it is tall

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Muller-Lyer illusion

Muller-Lyer illusion. The two lines with arrows at each end appear to be different lengths on the left. In fact they are the same length

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Illusory contours

Illusory contours forming circles. This illusion demonstrates the false perception of areas of increased brightness. Lines are apparently joined to bright circles. In fact no such circles exist

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Freemish crate

Freemish crate. This representation of an impossible cube is based upon the standard 2- dimensional depiction of a cube, but with some of the edges crossing in a way that could not be achieved in 3

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Jastrow illusion

Jastrow illusion. The bottom shape appears to be larger than the top shape. They are actually identical

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Circle in a square illusion

Circle in a square illusion. The two red circles in the image are the same size. However, the one inside a blue square looks larger than the one enclosing a blue square

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Hering illusion

Hering illusion. This illusion was created by the physiologist Edward Hering in the 19th century. It exploits the brains understanding of perspective to fool it into thinking that the two horizontal

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Framing colour illusion

Framing colour illusion
Framing colours illusion. Black borders around colours enhance their brightness and prevent clashing. The colours in the pattern on the left appear to be brighter and bolder than those on the right

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Poggendorff illusion

Poggendorff illusion. The two red lines crossing the blue bar are colinear. Many people see them as being offset. There are still no satisfactory explanations as to why this should be the case

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Three arc illusion

Three arc illusion. The three arcs shown give the impression of belonging to circles of increasing radius when viewed from top to bottom. They actually come from circles of the same radius

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Square illusion - orientation

Square illusion - orientation changes perception of size. The two squares are identical. However, the one on the left appears larger because it has been rotated through 45 degrees

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Kanizsa triangle

Kanizsa triangle. This image appears to depict a solid triangle overlaying another triangle. This triangle looks brighter than the background. In fact there is no triangle

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Delboeuf illusion

Delboeuf illusion. An illusion created by the Belgian philosopher Franz Joseph Delboeuf. The two red circles on the left have the same diameter, as do the two red circles on the right

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Computer artwork of an impossible triangle

Computer artwork of an impossible triangle
Impossible triangle. Abstract computer artwork of a triangle, made from 3 sections of tower block, which would be impossible to construct because it breaks the laws of artistic perspective

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Angle illusion

Angle illusion. The red angle marked on the left appears to be larger than the red angle on the right. In fact they are the same size

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Square illusion - vertical lines appear longer

Square illusion - vertical lines appear longer
Square illusion. The shape on the right hand side is a square whilst the one on the left, which is slightly shorter, is not. However many people believe it to be the other way round

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Orbison illusion

Orbison illusion. Both the rectangle surrounding the pattern and the perfect square within the pattern appear to be distorted

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Irradiation illusion

Irradiation illusion. The white square against a black background on the left appears to be larger than the black square against a white background on the right. They are actually the same size

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Cafe wall illusion

Cafe wall illusion. This illusion is so named because it was first noticed on a tiled wall in a Bristol cafe. The pattern in the image appears to show sloping grey lines between the black

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Simultaneous contrast

Simultaneous contrast example. The green squares look brighter on the left, where they are in yellow bands, compared to the right, where they are in blue bands

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Hermann-Hering illusion

Hermann-Hering illusion
Hermann grid illusion. The intersections of the white horizontal and vertical lines in this image appear to be grey. But when looked at directly they are clearly white

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Penrose stairway

Penrose stairway. This is an impossible figure created by the physicist Roger Penrose and used by M C Escher in his illustration " Ascending and Descending"

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Ehrenstein illusion

Ehrenstein illusion - illusory contours forming circles. Created in 1954 by the psychologist Walter Ehrenstein, this illusion demonstrates the false perception of areas of increased brightness

Background imageTrickery Vision Collection: Impossible figure, artwork

Impossible figure, artwork
Impossible figure. Computer artwork of a mug on a piece of paper, both with impossible figures drawn on them. Impossible figures are objects that can be drawn but not created



For sale as Licensed Images

Choose your image, Select your licence and Download the media

"Unveiling the Enigma: Dive into the World of Trickery Vision" Step into a realm where reality bends and perception deceives. The Goblet illusion, with its mesmerizing play of contours, challenges our visual senses to discern between foreground and background. Meanwhile, the Penrose stairs lure us into an infinite loop of ascending and descending steps, defying logic at every turn. Art becomes an accomplice in this game of trickery as well. Behold the Impossible triangle artwork that tantalizes our minds by presenting a geometric enigma impossible to exist in three dimensions. And who can resist being captivated by the Ouchi illusion's mind-bending dance of colors? The Ponzos illusion takes us on a journey where size distorts perspective; parallel lines converge or diverge depending on their surroundings—an optical riddle that leaves us questioning what we see. Similarly, the Ehrenstein illusion teases our eyes with squares seamlessly transforming into circles—a paradoxical blend of shapes that challenges our understanding. Prepare yourself for another twist as you encounter impossible shapes—figures that defy Euclidean geometry yet manage to deceive even the most astute observer. And let's not forget about the Square illusion – subdivision edition – where patterns within patterns create an intricate web of confusion. As we navigate through this world brimming with illusions, we stumble upon one more puzzling phenomenon—the Muller-Lyer illusion—where arrows seemingly alter their lengths based solely on their fin-like extensions. Trickery vision unveils itself through these captivating hints—a labyrinthine exploration where nothing is quite as it seems. So open your eyes wide and embrace this enchanting journey through optical deception.